The genesis and progression of language through cognitive expansion and creolization
Main Article Content
Abstract
This paper contains a scenario for the genesis, and part of the progression of language. It consists of a concise interdisciplinary explanation on how modern speech began, by tracing back to language’s first appearance through the attainment of physical aptitude for speech, and cognitive expansion of hominans. This is preceded by a short discourse on the linguistic record of archaic language users Homo sapiens and Homo neanderthalensis: a partially common record that can be outlined genetically as well as linguistically. Subsequent focus will be laid on how the current great extent of linguistic diversity may for a substantial part have developed through pidginization and creolization. It will be concluded that that since the utilitarian bond between Homo sapiens and Homo neanderthalensis that existed between 95-30 kya, the creolization process has been a common cause for language progression and a main reason for present language diversity.
Article Details
Statements and opinions expressed in articles herein are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the position of the editors or publisher.
Article, information, text, image, etc. which are published in Journal of International Studies, belong to Journal of International Studies. If anybody or any organization would like to use part or whole of them, they must receive written permission from Journal of International Studies before usage.
References
Amos, W. & Hoffman, J.I. (2010). Evidence that two main bottleneckevents shaped modern human genetic diversity. Proceedings: Biological siences, 277: 131-137.
Bailey, C. J. N., & Maroldt, K. (1977). The French lineage of English. In J. M. Meisel (Ed.), Pidgins – creoles – languages in contact. Tübingen: Narr.
Bickerton, D. (1984). The language bioprogram hypothesis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7: 173-221.
Bickerton, D. (1990). Language and species. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Bickerton, D. (1995). Language and human behavior. Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press.
Bickerton, D. (2000). How protolanguage became language. In C. Knight, M. Studdert- Kennedy, & J. R. Hurford (Eds.). The evolutionary emergence of language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Bickerton, D. (2002). Foraging versus social intelligence in the evolution of protolanguage. In A. Wray (Ed.), The transition to language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Bickerton, D. (2009). Adam’s tongue. New York, NY: Hill and Wang.
Boë. L-J., Heim, J-L., Honda, K., Maëda, S. (2002). The potential Neandertal vowel space was as large as that of modern humans. Journal of Phonetics 20: 465-484.
Chaudenson, R. (1992). Des îles, des hommes, des langues. Paris : l’Harmattan.
Chaudenson, R. (2001). Creolization of language and culture. London: Routledge.
Chomsky, N. (1995). The Minimalist Program, Cambridge: MIT Press.
Chomsky, N. (2007). Of minds and language. Biolinguistics 1: 9–27.
Clark, A. (1997). Being There: Putting Brain, Body, and World Together Again. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Coulmas, F. (2002). Writing systems: An introduction to their linguistic analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Coulmas, F. (2000). The writing systems of the world. Malden, Mass.: Blackwell.
Dediu D., and Levinson S.C., (2013). On the antiquity of language: the reinterpretation of Neandertal linguistic capacities and its consequences. Frontiers in Psychology 4:1-17.
De Groot, J.Y. (2012). Urak Lawoi’ grammar and social history. Phuket: Prince of Songkla University Press.
Fitch, W.T. (2009). Fossil cues to the evolution of speech. In Botha, R. & Khight, C. (Eds.), The Cradle of language; studies in the evolution of language. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press.
Foley, W.A. (1986). The Papuan languages of New Guinea. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Fu, Q. et al. (2013). A revised timescale for human evolution based on ancient mitochondrial genomes. Current Biology 23: 1-7.
Fu, Q., Li, H., et al. (2014) Genome sequence of a 45,000-year-old modern human from western Siberia. Nature 514: 445-450.
Goyette, S. (2000). The emergence of the Romance languages from Latin: a case for creolization effects. Ottawa: University of Ottawa.
Heine, B., & Kuteva, T. (2007). The genesis of grammar: A reconstruction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Henshilwood, C.S. & Mareau, C.W. (2003). The origin of modern human behavior - Critique of the models and their test implications. Current Anthropology 44/5: 627-651.
Hershkovitz, I; Marder, O.; Ayalon, A. et al. (2015). Levantine cranium from Manot Cave foreshadows the first modern Europeans. Nature Vol 00: 1-13 (doi: 10.1038_nature14134).
Holm J. (2004). An introduction to Pidgins and creoles. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Jackendoff, R. (1999). Possible stages in the evolution of the language capacity, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3: 272-279.
Kerswill, P. (2000). Koineization and accommodation. In: J. K. Cham-bers, P. Trudgill & N. Schilling-Estes (eds.)
The handbook of language variation and change. Oxford: Blackwell.
Lalueza-Fox, C., Rosas, A., Estalrrich, A. et al. (2010). Genetic evidence for patrilocal mating behavior among Neandertal groups. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108: 250–253.
Lefebvre, C. (2004). Issues in the study of pidgin and creole languages. Studies in language companion series 70. Amster-dam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
Lefebvre, C., & Loranger, V. (2006). On the properties of Saramaccan fu: Synchronic and diachronic perspectives. Journal of Pidgin and Creole Languages 21: 275–337.
Lefebvre, C. (2013). On the relevance of pidgins and creoles in the debate on the origins of language. In: Lefebvre, C., Comrie, B., Cohen, H. (eds), New Perspectives on the Origins of Language. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamin's Publishing Company.
Lieberman, P. & Crelin, E.S. (1971). On the speech of the Neandertal man. Linguistic Inquiry 2: 203-222.
Lim, L. (2011). Tone in Singlish: Substrate features from Sinitic and Malay. In: in Lefebvre, C. (ed). Creoles, their Substrates, and Language Typology. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
Luís, A.R. (2009). The loss and survival of inflectional morphology: Contextual vs inherent inflection in creoles. In: Romance linguistics 2009. Current issues in linguistic theory 135. Amster-dam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Martínez, I., Rosa, M., Arsuaga, J.-L. et al. (2004). Auditory capacities in Middle Pleistocene humans from the Sierra de Atapuerca in Spain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 101: 9976–9981.
McBrearty, S. & Brooks, A.S. (2000). The revolution that wasn’t: A new interpretation of the origins of human behavior. Journal of Human Evolution 39: 453-563.
McLarnon, A.M. & Hewitt, G.P. (1999). The evolution of human speech: The role of enhanced breathing control.
American Journal of Physical Anthropology 109: 341-363.
Mellars, P & French, J.C. (2011). Tenfold population increase in Western Europe at the Neandertal - to - Modern Human transition. Science 333: 623-627.
Mithen, S. (1996). The Prehistory of the Mind: A Search for the Origins of Art, Science and Religion. London: Thames and Hudson.
Odling-Smee, F. J. Laland, K. N. & Feldman M. W. (2003). Niche Construction: The Neglected Process in Evolution.
Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Pääbo, S. (2014). Neanderthal man: in search of lost genomes. Philadelphia: Perseus.
Pinker, S. & Bloom, P. (1990). Natural Language and Natural Selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13, pp 707-84.
Reinecke, J. E. et al. (1975). Languages of India: Pidginization/Creolization. Oceanic Linguistics Special
Publications, No. 14, A Bibliography of Pidgin and Creole Languages. pp. 632-635. Honolulu: University of Hawai'i Press.
Sangkakaraman, S., Pattersen, N., Li, H., Pääbo, S., Reich, D. (2012). The date of interbreeding between Neandertals and modern humans. PLoS Genetics. 8(10), e1002947.
Samovar, L.A., Porter, R. E., & McDaniel E. R. (2011). Intercultural Communication: A Reader. New York: Wadsworth Publishing.
Schrenk, F & Müller, S (2008). The Neanderthals. New York: Routledge.
Smith, A.D.M. (2008). Protolanguage reconstructed. Interaction Studies, 9, pp 100–116.
Smith, G.E. (1924). Evolution of man. New York: Oxford University Press.
Sneddon, J. (2006). Colloquial Jakartan Indonesian. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics.
Štekauer, P (2005). Onomasiological approach to word-formation. In: Štekauer, P. & Lieber, R. (eds.). Handbook of word-formation. Studies in Natural language and linguistic Theory Vol 64. Pp. 207-232. Dordrecht: Springer.
Sterelny, K. (2003), Thought in a Hostile World: The Evolution of Human Cognition, London, Blackwell.
Stringer, C. (2002). Modern human origins: Progress and prospects. Philosophical Transactions Biological Sciences Volume 357 issue 1420.
Tomasello, M. (2000). The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Tuggy, D. (2005). Cognitive approach to word-formation. In: Štekauer, P. & Lieber, R. (eds.). Handbook of word-formation. Studies in Natural language and linguistic Theory Vol 64. Pp. 233-265. Dordrecht: Springer.
Valdman, A. (1988). Ann pale Kreyol: An introductory course in Haitian Creole. Bloomington: Indiana University.
Verhaar, J.W.M. (1995). Toward a reference grammar of Tok Pisin: an experiment in corpus linguistics. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.
Versteegh, K. (2008). Non-Indo-European Pidgins and Creoles. In Kouwenberg, S. & Singler, J. V. (eds.). The Handbook of Pidgin and Creole Studies. Chichester: Blackwell Publishing Ltd.
Wurm, S. A. (1993). Some Contact Languages and Pidgin and Creole Languages in the Siberian Region. Language Sciences, Volume 14, Number 3, pp. 249-285.